Studies in Higher Education ISSN: 0307-5079 (Print) 1470-174X (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cshe20 Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills in higher education Tilly Mortimore & W. Ray Crozier To cite this article: Tilly Mortimore & W. Ray Crozier (2006) Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills in higher education, Studies in Higher Education, 31:2, 235-251, DOI: 10.1080/03075070600572173 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/03075070600572173 Published online: 24 Jan 2007. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 7178 View related articles Citing articles: 95 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=cshe20 Studies in Higher Education Vol. 31, No. 2, April 2006, pp. 235–251 Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills in higher education Tilly Mortimorea and W. Ray Crozierb* aUniversity of Southampton, UK; bCardiff University, UK 0W.RayCrozier School 00000April of Social 2006SciencesCardiff UniversityKing Edward VII AvenueCardiffCF 10 [email protected] Taylor Studies 10.1080/03075070600572173 CSHE_A_157200.sgm 0307-5079 Original Society 22006 31 and in for Article Higher (print)/1470-174X Francis Research Education Ltd into Higher (online) Education This article presents findings from a questionnaire survey of 136 male students, 62 with dyslexia and 74 without dyslexia, from 17 British higher education institutions. The students with dyslexia reported difficulties with a wide range of skills and academic tasks, notably note taking, organization of essays and expressing ideas in writing. They reported that their difficulties were long-standing and had been experienced in primary and secondary school, although the pattern of these difficulties changed over time. They reported making use of resources available to them, including additional time for examinations, access to dyslexia tutors and support with information technology. However, there are indications of unmet needs in several areas, notably support for specific subjects and with organizing coursework, learning in lectures, and academic writing skills. The implications of these findings for provision for students with dyslexia are discussed. Introduction The subject of dyslexia is controversial, and debates continue about the extent and nature of learning difficulties involved, their diagnosis, their causes and the form that any interventions should take. For example, a widely accepted definition of dyslexia offered by the World Federation of Neurologists as ‘a disorder manifested by difficulty in learning to read, despite conventional instruction, adequate intelligence and sociocultural opportunity’ (Critchley, 1999, p. 361), has attracted criticism for its narrow focus on learning to read, its emphasis on deficits and its neglect of the context in which literacy and numeracy skills are acquired. Alternative terminology exists where, for example, dyslexia is regarded as an instance of a ‘specific learning difficulty’ (Pumfrey & Reason, 1991). Notwithstanding controversies and uncertainties about terminology, dyslexia is acknowledged by higher education institutions in the UK as a form of disability, and the term is used to name student support units and in policies and provision for students with special circumstances, for example, in *Corresponding author: School of Social Sciences, Cardiff University, Glamorgan Building, King Edward VII Avenue, Cardiff CF10 3WT, UK. Email: [email protected] ISSN 0307-5079 (print)/ISSN 1470-174X (online)/06/020235–17 © 2006 Society for Research into Higher Education DOI: 10.1080/03075070600572173 236 T. Mortimore and W. R. Crozier examination regulations that allow additional time for students with dyslexia. There is growing recognition of the rights and needs of students with disabilities and this provides a context for the investigation reported here into the experiences of students with dyslexia in a sample of universities in the UK. Dyslexia represents the most frequent self-declared disability in higher education in the UK: for example, it was identified by 41% of first-year students declaring a disability in 2003–04 (Higher Education Statistics Agency, 2005). Precise statistics on the numbers of students with dyslexia in higher education in the UK are difficult to obtain, although it is evident that the numbers have been increasing substantially in recent years. For example, data provided by the Higher Education Statistics Agency (2005) show that the number of students in their first year of study who identified themselves as having dyslexia increased from 4304 in 1996–97 to 18,700 in 2003–04, an increase in the proportion of the student population from 0.7% to 2.18%. These statistics underestimate the prevalence of dyslexia to an unknown extent, since they do not include students who have difficulties similar to those who have dyslexia, but who have not been diagnosed as such, or students who have dyslexia but choose not to identify themselves to their university. Nevertheless, despite the limitations of the available data, there are significant numbers of students with dyslexia in higher education, and there is increasing awareness of the importance of assessing their needs (Fuller et al., 2004; Teachability, 2000). Recent legislation in the UK has provided an impetus to universities to review their policies and practices with regard to provision for students with dyslexia. The Special Educational Needs and Disability Act of 2001 (SENDA) extended the 1995 Disability Discrimination Act to higher education and its provisions relating to teaching and learning of students with disabilities came into effect in September 2002. This will affect the experience of students (and universities’ record keeping, and therefore data on the prevalence of dyslexia). The questionnaire data reported here were collected between March 2002 and the summer of 2003, soon after the implementation of SENDA; the findings with regard to university provision may be different in future years when any changes following SENDA have had an impact. The universities participating in this study were asked about the provision they made for students with dyslexia, so that student responses could be compared with provision existing at that time. It is clear that many students embark on degree courses with severe problems in acquiring and employing a range of skills that would in the past have been regarded as essential for effective study at this level. The scope of the problems that students experience is illustrated by a definition provided by the UK Dyslexia Institute of specific learning difficulties ‘as organizing or learning deficiencies which restrict the students’ competencies in information processing, in motor skills and working memory, so causing limitations in some or all of the skills of speech, reading, spelling, writing, essay writing, numeracy and behaviour’ (cited by Pumfrey & Reason, 1991, p. 14). Consistent with this definition, the research reported here does not assume that students’ difficulties are restricted to reading, spelling and writing, but may be experienced across the range of tasks that students encounter in higher education. Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills 237 Most research into dyslexia has concentrated on the difficulties experienced by children, yet applying concepts and methods that may be appropriate for the school years to learning in higher education is problematic, not least because students have developed strategies that have enabled them to attain sufficiently high grades in school to meet the admission criteria for university. The experiences of students with dyslexia in higher education warrant research in their own right. It has been argued that research into dyslexia, at least in childhood, has been dominated by what might be termed the deficit-diagnosis-remediation model of dyslexia, and that this needs to be replaced by a ‘personal empowerment’ model that seeks to find ways to support learners in the pursuit of their goals (Hunter-Carsch & Herrington, 2001; Fuller et al., 2004). In order to support learners effectively it is important to analyze the ways in which the policies and practices of higher education institutions facilitate or place obstacles in the way of empowerment, to investigate the experiences of students with dyslexia and to identify their needs. The identification of students’ perceived difficulties and needs for support is the principal aim of the study reported here. It is important for research to compare the perceived difficulties and needs of students with dyslexia with those who have not been so diagnosed, since higher education makes demands upon all students, and the difficulties experienced by those with dyslexia should be evaluated within this context. Exploration of this issue is the second aim of this study. Finally, the study aims to investigate the learning difficulties that this sample of students report having experienced prior to university, in order to examine how long-standing their difficulties have been and whether, and in what ways, students believe these have changed as they have progressed through the educational system. Some published guidelines on study skills aimed at students with dyslexia indicate the kinds of problems that are encountered. Klein (1993) listed persistent difficulties: memorizing names and facts, remembering sequences, rote memory tasks, problems with telling the time and time-keeping, concentration, writing, copying and word retrieval (putting ideas on paper, either as lecture notes or when writing assignments). McLoughlin et al. (1994) reported students’ difficulties with verbal and written communication, reading, spelling, organization of work and concentration. Gilroy and Miles (1996) reported problems with time management and organization, reading, note taking, and writing assignments and examination answers. Riddick et al. (1997) report difficulties in retaining the meaning of texts, marshalling facts effectively in examinations, and producing disjointed written work due to losing track of sequential information. A systematic survey of the barriers to learning reported by students with dyslexia, within a sample of students with disabilities in one university, identified problems with learning from lectures, using learning centres and libraries, and producing coursework assignments (Fuller et al., 2004). Reading continues to pose problems in higher education, as even students who have developed effective coping strategies, and who score adequately on standardized reading tests, can experience difficulty with the higher levels of fluency required to read university texts. Some students report problems with motor skills and coordination, so that they find it difficult to produce legible script or to master keyboard skills (Nicolson & Fawcett, 1990). 238 T. Mortimore and W. R. Crozier Richardson and Wydell (2003) provided evidence that students with dyslexia encountered difficulties with their programme of study and obtained lower attainments: they were more likely to withdraw after their first year and were less likely to obtain a ‘good degree’ (a first class or upper second class degree). Problems with academic work can have an impact on self-esteem and selfconfidence, particularly when written assignments attract criticism for their poor presentation and weaknesses of grammar, punctuation and spelling (see, for example, Kavale & Forness, 1996). Many tutors will be ignorant of a student’s diagnosis and, even when they are aware of it, their marking may be guided by the belief that, whatever the student’s difficulties, the effective production of written work is an essential component of ‘graduateness’. Alternatively, tutors’ assessment of the content may be influenced unconsciously by the poor quality of presentation. Some students with dyslexia report feelings of inferiority and lack of confidence (McLoughlin et al., 1994) and anxiety about assessment (Fuller et al., 2004). The aim of this research is to survey the perceived difficulties and support needs of a sample of students who have been diagnosed with dyslexia, and to compare their responses with those of a sample of students not identified as having dyslexia. The study focuses exclusively on male students. The data reported here were collected in the course of an investigation into the influence of dyslexia and cognitive style upon recall of material in a simulated lecture task, and adding gender as a factor in that study would have produced design and sample size problems. Gender is a pertinent issue for dyslexia. Research suggests that dyslexia is more common among males (Pumfrey & Reason, 1991; Snowling, 2000), and that more male students identify themselves as having dyslexia (National Working Party on Dyslexia in Higher Education, 1999). However, Shaywitz et al. (1990) suggest that this trend might be due more to diagnostic techniques and gender differences in responses to the condition, than to any underlying difference in incidence, while Richardson and Wydell (2003) argue that the evidence suggests that the gender difference in higher education has been overestimated. These issues are not addressed in this study. A questionnaire was devised specifically for the study, drawing upon the difficulties and needs identified in the research summarized above, and also those that emerged from a pilot study. The items refer to students’ experiences of learning difficulties prior to and during higher education. They also explore perceptions of the learning support available at their higher education institutions. Additional items question the students about their preferred learning strategies in higher education. Sample selection provides practical and ethical problems for the researcher into dyslexia in higher education, problems that are magnified when different institutions of higher education are included in the sample. There is the problem of identifying students with dyslexia. One approach would be to advertise throughout each university in a search for volunteers. However, students might be reluctant to identify themselves as ‘dyslexic’, or might be reluctant to commit themselves to participation, particularly if they are experiencing the difficulties keeping abreast of their study commitments that the research is intended to investigate. Furthermore, this approach Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills 239 would raise the issue for the researcher of how to establish whether the participants did have dyslexia, with the assessment and ethical problems this would raise. Alternatively, potential participants could be approached through their university centres for student support or dedicated dyslexia units. This is the approach adopted here. The researcher does not ask to be provided with any contact information, but sends letters to potential participants via these centres. This has the advantages of maintaining student anonymity, placing little direct pressure on them to participate, and approaching them about dyslexia via a channel that is relevant to their needs. This procedure has its limitations. It can introduce bias, since it excludes those who were not aware of their dyslexia, who had not yet been diagnosed as having dyslexia, or who, for whatever reason, had chosen to conceal their dyslexia from the university. Furthermore, it means that the researcher is dependent upon the cooperation and support of the staff in these centres and units, and any unwillingness to participate in the projects will have a significant impact on the representativeness of the sample. This approach also raises the issue of the operational definition of dyslexia, since the researcher is dependent on the criteria that are adopted by each higher education institution, and can expect that the students will have been diagnosed by a large number of professionals applying a range of different tests and procedures. While the research reported here is guided by the working definition of dyslexia quoted above, in practice the operational definition of dyslexia must be in terms of procedures adopted by institutions and agencies that are involved with the participating students. All the participating students identified as having dyslexia had a diagnosis of dyslexia based upon a psychometric assessment by an educational psychologist. Method Participants The participating institutions comprised four ‘old’ universities and 13 ‘new’ universities. In the UK the term ‘new university’ refers to the former polytechnics and colleges that obtained university status following the Further and Higher Education Act of 1992. The distinction between old universities, those having university status prior to 1992, and the new universities has persisted, in part due to differences in funding allocations and in the entry qualifications of students. An attempt was made to include students from as wide a range of degree programmes as possible, with representation from business studies, creative arts, drama, medicine, music and social studies. The sample comprised 136 male students from 17 universities located in Wales, the west of England, and London. Sixty-two of these students were diagnosed with dyslexia and a comparison group of 74 students without diagnosis were matched as closely as possible with the dyslexia group for age, academic subject studied, and year of study. Members of the comparison group were contacted at the same universities as members of the dyslexia group, and were recruited in a number of ways, including asking the student with dyslexia to bring along a fellow student from their degree programme. A list was drawn up of the programmes followed by students with 240 T. Mortimore and W. R. Crozier dyslexia, and individual students without dyslexia on those programmes were approached and asked to participate. No inducements were offered for participation, although each student was promised feedback from his performance on the computer-administered cognitive styles assessment. Potential participants were not screened for disabilities, but the questionnaire asked participants about any disabilities. None of the comparison sample reported dyslexia, but one reported a physical disability. Although the absolute numbers of students with dyslexia in the sample were comparable in the two types of institutions (29 in old universities, and 33 in new universities), the composition of the two groups varied, with students with dyslexia comprising 35% of the old universities sample and 63% of the new universities sample. This imbalance in representation of the types of universities reflects practical difficulties in obtaining a matched sample, and is taken into account in the analyses presented below. Questionnaire The questionnaire comprised several sections. The first section asked respondents to indicate any experience of difficulties with the aspects of learning described below and the stage of education (primary school, secondary school [including further education], university) when these difficulties had affected them. They were instructed to give a ‘yes’ response if they could remember having difficulty with a particular aspect. The difficulties were grouped as follows: reading; spelling; note taking; organization and time keeping; general study skills; expressing ideas orally; handwriting; memory and concentration (Table 1 lists the items). Table 1. Percentages of dyslexia and comparison groups reporting difficulties in higher education % Dyslexia % Comparison Chi-square, dyslexia vs. comparison % Dyslexia Old Universities % Dyslexia New Universities 47 64 62 78 76 67 55 24 72 64 52 57 40 6 7 7 18 8 36 31 10 11 18 25 14 12 29.73 *** 47.47 *** 45.20 *** 46.07 *** 61.89 *** 12.45 *** 8.02 ** 4.93 * 51.00 *** 28.39 *** 9.85 ** 26.84 *** 13.29 *** 54 61 54 71 71 64 57 25 64 64 43 54 36 40 67 70 83 80 70 53 23 80 63 60 60 43 Reading Reading speed Spelling Note taking Organizing essays General organization Time keeping Expressing ideas orally Expressing ideas in writing Handwriting Concentration Remembering facts Listening *p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001. Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills 241 The second section of the questionnaire comprised items referring to support needs. Items were based on a list of learning support provisions that was compiled after consultation with the learning support facilitators in a number of the universities participating in the project. The items were grouped into the following categories (see Table 3 for items): examination assessment methods; tutorial support; group support; information technology (IT) support; coursework support; lecture support; vision assessment; counselling facilities. Students were asked to respond to each item by indicating whether they had used, would like to use, or had no need to use each of these forms of support. Students were invited to mention any other form of learning support that had not been included in the set of items. Further sections of the questionnaire asked students to indicate if they had a diagnosis of dyslexia, dyspraxia (disorder of organization of movement), attention deficit disorder or any other learning difficulty, and to state at what stage of education this had been established. In the final section, respondents were provided with the opportunity to add any additional comments about their learning needs. Because most of the respondents (and all of the respondents with dyslexia except for two) completed the questionnaire individually rather than in a group setting, the researcher was able to invite the students to clarify and elaborate upon their responses; these contributions were written verbatim and later transferred to a file for subsequent content analysis. Procedure Seventeen universities in South Wales and the west of England (regions close to the researchers’ base) and London (where one of the researchers is also based) were approached and all agreed to participate. The disability or dyslexia resource centre at each institution was asked if it would be willing to participate in the study by mailing a letter to all students who were registered with the centre as having dyslexia. The letter explained the context of the study and invited students to volunteer their participation by replying to the researcher, giving contact information and brief details of their age, year, and programme of study. Full confidentiality and anonymity were assured and, because their replies would be sent direct to the researcher, their resource centre would not know whether they had participated or had declined to do so. When positive replies were received, the students were contacted for an appointment. The brief details were used to approach students to participate in the comparison group. Across the two groups as a whole the participants were matched for programme and year. It was not, however, possible to ensure that each student with dyslexia attended the same university as their counterpart matched for programme. A quiet room was set aside for the project. When students arrived, they participated in the lecture simulation task, undertook a cognitive style assessment (these activities are not discussed further here) and completed the questionnaire that is the focus of this article. The order of these activities was randomized across participants. The disability or dyslexia resource centres in the 17 institutions were also asked to indicate which of the support facilities and resources listed in the questionnaire were 242 T. Mortimore and W. R. Crozier available to students in the academic years 2001–02 and 2002–03, the years covered by this research. Ten of the centres responded to this request, representing 117 of the 136 participants, including 40 of the 62 students with dyslexia. Results The responses to the questionnaire were coded, entered into an SPSS data file, and checked for transcription errors. The distributions of all variables were examined, and inspected for the presence of any outliers. Reported difficulties The frequencies of students with and without dyslexia reporting each type of difficulty in university were computed and are reported in Table 1. The difficulties most frequently endorsed by the students in the dyslexia group were with note taking, organizing essays and expressing ideas in writing. Nevertheless, all the other items were frequently endorsed, and only three difficulties were reported by less than half of the sample: reading (47%), listening (40%) and expressing ideas orally (24%). The pattern of responses of students with dyslexia attending old universities and new universities was compared. Inspection of the relevant data, presented in the two right-hand columns of Table 1, reveals that the overall pattern is comparable between the two types. There was, however, a tendency for more students with dyslexia in new universities to report difficulties with spelling, expressing ideas in writing and concentration. A series of chi-square analyses comparing the distribution of the responses of students with dyslexia and the comparison group confirmed that the students with dyslexia were significantly more likely to endorse each type of difficulty. Most of the items were endorsed by small numbers of students in the comparison group: reading, reading speed, spelling and organizing essays were all reported by less than 10% of the group. Nevertheless, there were some difficulties that were frequent in the comparison group: general organization (36%), time keeping (31%) and concentration (25%). Difficulties at successive stages of education Participants were also asked to indicate whether they had experienced any of the difficulties at primary and secondary school. Table 2 displays the frequencies of students in the dyslexia group endorsing each learning difficulty at each of the three stages of education identified. Rates of reporting difficulties are high across all levels. The most prominent difficulties reported in primary school were with spelling, reading, reading speed and handwriting. Although some of these decrease somewhat in frequency across the stages, they remain problems for substantial numbers of university students with dyslexia. The most marked changes take place between primary and secondary education, with substantial increases in the numbers reporting difficulties with note Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills 243 Table 2. Percentages of students in the dyslexia group reporting difficulties at three stages of education Reading Reading speed Spelling Note taking Organizing essays General organization Time keeping Expressing ideas orally Expressing ideas in writing Handwriting Concentration Remembering facts Listening Primary Secondary Higher 60 62 74 36 34 43 28 15 47 62 55 43 38 52 62 76 59 64 53 43 21 71 65 62 64 43 47 64 62 78 76 67 55 24 72 64 52 57 40 taking, organizing essays, time keeping, expressing ideas in writing and remembering facts. Note taking and organizing essays show further increases between secondary and higher education, and these are, along with expressing ideas in writing, the most cited difficulties at university. Rates of endorsement of difficulties were low in the comparison group, particularly at primary level, where the most frequent responses were to items referring to handwriting (19%), spelling and concentration (each 18%) and reading speed (17%). These difficulties become less pronounced over time, for example, in the cases of spelling (reduced from 18% and 19% in the primary and secondary school years respectively to 7% in university) and reading speed (from 17% and 14% to 7%), although the rates for handwriting remain constant at 18%. These decreases presumably represent perceptions of increasing skill and, in the case of spelling, access to word processors. Nevertheless, there are increases in difficulties across the stages of education that show parallel trends to those reported by the students with dyslexia, with increases between primary and secondary school in those noting organizing essays, general organization, time keeping and concentration as problematic. There are further increases between secondary school and university in note taking, general organization and time keeping. In summary, students with dyslexia report substantially more difficulties with all of the learning skills and tasks during primary, secondary and higher education. Both groups report increases across these stages in organization, time keeping and note taking, trends that presumably reflect the changing demands of teaching and learning methods, for example, increases in self-directed learning. Some difficulties that intensify markedly for students with dyslexia, expressing ideas in writing, for example, remain consistently low in the comparison group (10% of this group). Some difficulties that are reported by the comparison group in primary school—reading speed and spelling—reduce over time, but persist at a high level for the students with dyslexia. 244 T. Mortimore and W. R. Crozier Support needs The second section of the questionnaire asked participants about their support needs. Table 3 summarizes the percentages of students in the two groups who reported that they had used, would like to use or had no need to use each type of support. A series of chi-square analyses found that the difference in the distribution of responses between the two groups was statistically significant for every item, with 25 of the 31 differences significant beyond the .001 level. Despite their greater use relative to the comparison group, the students with dyslexia reported less use than might have been expected of the potential resources. Only two forms of support were reported by more than half of this group: extra time in examinations and the use of a scanner to scan text and images into a personal computer. The remaining resources that were most commonly used (reported by more than one in three respondents) were: a separate room for examinations; access to dyslexia tutors for help with literacy; access to individual tutors employed through the Disabled Student’s Allowance (DSA; additional funding for students with disabilities); open access to special computers, including those with speech–text conversion facility; access to voice-activated technology; help with structuring essays; the provision of lecture notes. There were several areas where a substantial proportion of students in the dyslexia group reported that they had not used a resource but would like to use it. Access to academic subject-specific support was used by only 13% of the group, but was thought to be desirable by 64%. The provision of taped lecture notes was used by only 3% but 64% would like to have this; 12% had used extra library support but another 50% wanted this. The remaining items in the coursework support and lecture support categories were used by between 25 and 41% of respondents, but were thought desirable by between 46% and 60% of them, with the numbers wishing to use the resource more frequent than those who had used it in each case. Several items related to examinations were reported as unnecessary by a substantial proportion of respondents in the dyslexia group: a reader (81%), the use of an amanuensis (66%), use of a word processor, tape-recording of answers, the opportunity to be examined orally, and use of coloured overlays (all thought unnecessary by more than 50%). Tutorial assistance in the form of study skills and untrained support tutors were also reported as unnecessary by a majority of respondents, as were support groups. A clearer picture of unmet needs is obtained if we consider, for each item, those students who have expressed a need for a resource (that is, all those who did not endorse the ‘no need’ option) and calculate the proportion of these who have used the resource. The outcome of this analysis is displayed in Table 4 for those resources where there is a clear expression of need (defined as a need reported by at least 75% of respondents with dyslexia). Only three needs are being met for a majority of these students: extra time in examinations, access to literacy-based tutors and access to a scanner. Only 17% report receiving the subject-specific support they would like, and support is lacking in several other areas: organizing coursework, providing copies of Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills 245 Table 3. Percentages of participants with and without dyslexia reporting learning support needs Dyslexia group Comparison group Have Would No need Have Would No need used like to use to use used like to use to use Exam assessment Extra time allocated Amanuensis (writer) Reader to read exam questions Use of word processor in exam Tape recording of answers Opportunity for oral examination Use of coloured overlays Separate room – alternative venue Tutorial support Dyslexia tutors (literacy based) Subject-specific support Study skills tutors Untrained support tutors Individual support tutors employed via DSA Group Activities Fellow student ‘study buddies’ Support groups Study skills or literacy support workshops IT support Open access to special computers Scanners Voice activated technology Support from trained technicians Coursework support Structuring essays Academic writing skills Grammar, punctuation Organizing coursework Extra library support Lecture support Tape recording in lectures Lecture notes provided Taped lectures provided Copies of overheads provided Other Vision assessment Counselling facilities 62 16 11 12 12 5 18 47 18 18 8 32 32 39 29 16 20 66 81 56 56 56 53 37 2 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 16 4 1 10 10 13 7 6 82 96 99 90 90 86 92 94 49 13 12 21 36 30 64 32 20 31 21 23 56 59 33 1 15 0 3 0 4 11 10 1 3 95 74 90 96 97 26 4 16 37 38 44 37 58 40 29 4 3 12 14 10 59 82 87 39 55 36 28 43 27 36 42 18 18 28 30 7 20 0 26 8 20 18 17 85 60 82 57 41 28 32 25 12 47 54 46 57 50 12 18 22 18 38 20 10 4 13 9 20 21 23 34 10 60 79 73 53 81 30 38 3 28 48 55 64 60 22 7 33 12 0 48 0 35 24 21 24 32 76 31 76 33 22 16 38 32 40 52 4 3 6 7 90 90 246 T. Mortimore and W. R. Crozier Table 4. Percentage of students with dyslexia who report the need for a resource and who have used that resource Percent report needing resource Percent of these who have used resource 80 78 79 77 62 17 IT support Open access to special computers Scanners 82 82 48 67 Coursework support Structuring essays Academic writing skills Grammar, punctuation Organizing coursework 88 82 78 82 47 34 41 30 Lecture support Tape recording in lectures Lecture notes provided Copies of overheads provided 78 93 88 38 41 32 Exam assessment Extra time allocated Tutorial support Dyslexia tutors (literacy based) Subject-specific support transparencies in lectures, academic writing skills, tape-recording of lectures, provision of printed lecture notes and help with grammar and punctuation. In order to interpret unambiguously the extent of use of a resource, it is essential to establish whether the resource is indeed available to potential users. Inspection of the questionnaires completed by staff at the disability and dyslexia resource centres confirmed that all reported meeting their statutory requirements, and offered support in terms of provision of extra time and access to information technology in examinations, availability of literacy based dyslexia tutors, access to specialist IT and scanners, and availability of extra library support. All claimed to provide standard course-related support for academic writing skills, organization skills and study skills tuition. They claimed to make available tape-recording of lectures, printed lecture notes and copies of transparencies. It would appear that the lack of take-up of resources is not a matter of unavailability, but of increasing students’ awareness of what is available and overcoming barriers to take-up, whether these originate in the institution’s system or among the students. Turning to the comparison group, very few students in this group had made use of learning support for examinations or tutorial support related to study skills. However, a substantial minority of this group (between 20% and 34%) expressed the desire to use some of the resources. These tended to be related to coursework support, such as organizing coursework, structuring essays, academic writing skills, grammar and Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills 247 punctuation; and to areas related to extracting and retaining information from lectures by means of the provision of copies of overheads, notes and taped lectures and facilities for tape-recording lectures. Nevertheless, the proportion of this group who reported that they would like each of these resources was significantly smaller than the proportion in the group with dyslexia. Discussion Although the students with dyslexia who participated in this survey were sufficiently well qualified to obtain entry to higher education, and were meeting the academic standards of their institutions, they reported considerably more difficulties with a range of learning and study skills than did a sample of their peers without dyslexia. The difficulties reported by the group with dyslexia are most pronounced in the areas of note taking, organizing essays and expressing ideas in writing. However, all of the remaining difficulties included in the questionnaire were frequently endorsed, indicating that these students encounter widespread problems in their studying. The success of students with dyslexia is clearly hard won. The comparison with a sample of students who do not have dyslexia is essential, since it would be problematical to gauge the extent of difficulties of students with dyslexia without taking into account those reported by their peers. The responses from the comparison group show that a substantial minority of this group report difficulties with organization, time keeping and concentration. Nevertheless, on all items the rates are considerably lower than those found among the dyslexia group. The pattern is broadly similar for students with dyslexia attending old and new universities, although students in the latter report more difficulties in the areas of expressing ideas in writing, concentration, spelling and note taking. This may reflect differences in the nature of degree programmes in the two types of universities. Alternatively, new institutions are more likely to recruit students from non-traditional or ‘access’ routes, who might have more difficulties with study skills. However, comparison of difficulties reported by the comparison groups in the two types of institutions reveals few differences. The only item endorsed by more students in new universities (17% compared to 9%) referred to expressing ideas in writing. This suggests that the difference related to type of university is specific to students with dyslexia in the sample, rather than a reflection of general differences across the university sector. Difficulties go beyond the areas where support is conventionally offered—for example, provision of extra time in examinations—to include essay organization, the ability to express ideas in writing, and note taking. The last is a common concern expressed in the comments made by respondents. A similar picture emerges when the reported support needs of students with dyslexia are analyzed, with students expressing need for support with essay writing, organization of coursework and note-taking in lectures. Lack of confidence in note-taking and in retention of factual information has been described in previous research (Klein, 1993; Riddick, et al., 1997; Farmer, et al., 2002). Fuller et al. (2004) reported that virtually all the students with dyslexia in their sample of students with disabilities from one British university reported 248 T. Mortimore and W. R. Crozier difficulties with learning in lectures: taking notes while listening and watching; lecturers talking too quickly or removing transparencies before the student could digest the information. Additional time for examinations and provision of a separate venue are common forms of support for students with disabilities, and there was evidence of take-up of these in this sample. Other forms of support were used by a substantial proportion of respondents, for example, access to dyslexia tutors and to information technology. Nevertheless, there are indications of unmet needs—where a substantial number express a need for a form of support but few have availed themselves of that form— in several areas, notably in coursework and lectures, and in subject-specific support. The centres that responded to the questionnaire claimed that the resources for which students expressed a need were available in their institution, and this implies shortcomings in communications between institutions and students. Fuller et al. (2004) reached a similar conclusion in their recommendations for good practice, following analysis of students’ perceptions of obstacles to learning in one university. Inspection of the comments made by participants shows that individual students have particular patterns of needs, and a flexible form of provision of support is essential. While students praised the quality of assistance generally offered by support units, they expressed frustration at delays in having their needs recognized by the university (delays of six months to one year are reported). One student reported on the delay: I declared my dyslexia on entry in the first year. It took till the end of the second year to get it sorted. Students expressed frustration at the lack of communication between the unit and the academic departments. Academic tutors frequently had little knowledge about dyslexia: ‘The lecturers need more workshops/training on how to deal with people with dyslexia’. Members of learning support teams were seen to lack the status of academic staff, a perception exacerbated by the gap between the department and unit. One student labelled this gap as a ‘glass wall’. Another student provided an example of the breakdown in communication between departments: In my exam, the lecturer didn’t realise I had extra time. In the hall, in public, he said, ‘Put your bloody pen down’. I had to say ‘I’m sorry I’m dyslexic’. It made me feel like a total leper. There is a lack of communication between departments, some know, some don’t. Although students were unanimous in their appreciation of the staff in support units, they did express criticisms of the lack of staff available, overworked members of staff, a dearth of equipment, limited opening hours and difficulty of access—several students reported that initially they didn’t know where to go or could not find the unit. Others expressed concern about the stigma of entering a unit labelled ‘Disability’. Several students commented that this made the initial approach difficult, because they did not feel that disability was part of their identity and because they did not want to become publicly labelled as member of a dyslexic or disabled ‘group’: I don’t use the centre because it’s all a bit alcoholics anonymous—I don’t want to go and join a group and whine about being dyslexic. Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills 249 Nine students stated that they had chosen not to take up the support available, while some mentioned friends whom they suspected might have dyslexia but who could not be persuaded to go for assessment. The stigma of dyslexia is a recurrent theme, which influenced the take-up of support and provoked anxieties about approaching tutors within academic departments who were not aware of the student’s status. Concerns were expressed about others’ preconceptions of dyslexia, and the judgements that are made about work characterized by poor presentation, organization, grammar and spelling. Students were anxious that they would be perceived as lacking in intelligence and had concerns about employability. These apprehensions create conflict over using support facilities: I don’t think they took my dyslexia into account for coursework. I am not sure if my tutor knew. I am not sure if I would have wanted others to inform him … I don’t go around telling stuff about my dyslexia because people who don’t understand might judge me differently if they don’t know what it is. I wouldn’t want a future employer to know as I think it might play against me. Students also reported worries about other students’ perceptions of the allowances that are made: ‘I always worry that other students will see my dyslexia as a free ticket to easy street’. On the other hand, fellow students were commonly mentioned as a form of support, for example, proof reading assignments and lending lecture notes. In summary, the academic success achieved by students with dyslexia masks their concerns about difficulties across a wide range of academic skills, tasks and activities. Fundamental elements of studying, such as organization and planning, attending to lectures, taking notes, reading and producing written work, cause problems, and mean that a considerable effort has to be invested in order to keep up with fellow students. The difficulties have an impact on student identity, with concerns about the stigma of disability and the potential for unpleasant interactions with staff. Respondents express their need for support in a number of areas and they identify shortcomings in the provision available, notably the time delay in official recognition and establishment of arrangements for their difficulties, and the lack of communication between academic departments and support units. They comment on the staffing levels of these units and, while this study has examined only one perspective on this issue, that of a sample of students diagnosed with dyslexia, this issue should concern universities as they plan to meet the requirements of disability legislation. The findings have specific implications for institutions. Tutors should recognize that the difficulties that students with dyslexia experience are not restricted to reading and spelling and are not overcome simply by provision of access to word processors with grammar and spelling checks. There has to be effective coordination among support units, academic departments and those units in central administration with responsibility for assessment, and the official recognition of special needs for examinations purposes. A substantial proportion of students with dyslexia are not taking advantage of the support that is available, and universities should aim to improve take-up. This requires the identification of factors that deter students from doing so 250 T. Mortimore and W. R. Crozier and strategies for surmounting these. This can be as straightforward as providing incoming students with explicit details about the location of support units, and ensuring that signs do not convey a message of disability. Universities have to be aware of the sensitivities surrounding dyslexia and the history of difficulties experienced by many students. More generally, policies should be guided by an empowerment model (Hunter-Carsch & Herrington, 2001) that acknowledges the strengths of students, recognizing their motivation and persistence in overcoming obstacles to their studies, and seeks to provide support for these. Finally, the study has identified concerns about study skills among the comparison group, which suggests universities also have a wider role in student support. The study has limitations, notably in the sampling of students. A more representative picture of students’ difficulties and support needs would require more systematic sampling across a large number of universities. This sample may be biased towards the inclusion of those students with dyslexia who are more concerned about their difficulties, or who are more interested in learning about them. Nevertheless, the participants with dyslexia are all registered with university support units, and they represent 17 higher education institutions in different parts of Britain, pursuing a variety of degree programmes. Furthermore, the differences between these students and the comparison sample are substantial. The focus on male students is clearly a serious limitation, not just in terms of reducing the representativeness of findings, but in the neglect of problems that might be specific to female students with dyslexia, or to such students in some institutions or degree programmes. Furthermore, the composition of the sample might unwittingly reinforce the perception that dyslexia is exclusively a male problem, a perception that is not warranted by findings on the gender distribution of students with dyslexia (Richardson & Wydell, 2003). While the findings about male students are valuable, a priority for future research should be to assess the experiences of female students. In conclusion, the findings suggest that students with dyslexia encounter difficulties with a wide range of study skills and learning tasks and these problems warrant attention by universities as they fulfil their obligations to students with disabilities. References Critchley, M. (1999) Specific developmental dyslexia, in E. H. Lenneberg & E. Lenneberg (Eds) Foundations of language development: a multidisciplinary approach (vol. 2) (New York, Academic Press). Farmer, M., Riddick, B. & Sterling, C. (2002) Dyslexia and inclusion: assessment and support for subject teachers (London, Whurr). Fuller, M., Healey, M., Bradley, A. & Hall, T. (2004) Barriers to learning: a systematic study of the experience of disabled students in one university, Studies in Higher Education, 29, 303–318. Gilroy, D. E. & Miles, T. R. (1996) Dyslexia at college (2nd edn) (London, Whurr). Higher Education Statistics Agency (2005) Student tables: Table 11b—first year UK domiciled HE students by qualification aim (#12), mode of study, gender and disability 2003/04. Available online at: http://www.hesa.ac.uk/holisdocs/pubinfo/student/disab0304.htm (accessed 15 May 2005) Hunter-Carsch, M. & Herrington, M. (2001) Dyslexia and effective learning in secondary and tertiary education (London, Whurr). Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills 251 Kavale, K. A. & Forness, S. R. (1996) Social skill deficits and learning disabilities: a meta-analysis, Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 226–237. Klein, C. (1993) Diagnosing dyslexia (London, Avanti). McLoughlin, D., Fitzgibbon, G. & Young, V. (1994) Adult dyslexia: assessment, counselling and training (London, Whurr). National Working Party on Dyslexia in Higher Education (1999) Dyslexia in higher education: policy, provision and practice (Hull, University of Hull). Nicolson, R. & Fawcett, A. (1990) Automaticity, a new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition, 35, 159–182. Pumfrey, P. D. & Reason, R. (1991) Specific learning difficulties. (Dyslexia) Challenges and responses (London, Routledge). Richardson, J. T. E. & Wydell, T. N. (2003) The representation and attainment of students with dyslexia in UK higher education, Reading and Writing, 16, 475–503. Riddick, B., Farmer, M. & Sterling, C. (1997) Students and dyslexia: growing up with a specific learning difficulty (London, Whurr). Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M. & Escobar, M. D. (1990). Prevalence of reading disorders in boys and girls: results of the Connecticut Longitudinal Study, Journal of American Medical Association, 264, 998–1002. Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd edn) (Oxford, Blackwell). Teachability (2000) Creating an accessible curriculum for students with disabilities (Glasgow, University of Strathclyde). Available online at: www.teachability.strath.ac.uk/index.html (accessed 15 May 2005).